V1 responses are enhanced with visual attention of various forms. An appealing hypothesis about how this happens is: Top-down inputs activate VIP interneurons, which disinhibits pyramidal cells through SOM cell inhibition & causes enhanced/modified responses to the same stimuli.

Ideally to test this, you would need to make an animal do an attention task, find attentionally modulated cells, and transiently switch off or switch on local VIP cells while simultaneously looking at responses of the attentionally modulated pyramidal cells.
And since any circuit manipulation has complicated effects on different cell classes, ideally you should know how multiple different cell classes respond to attention AND to the VIP manipulation in the same tissue (e.g. pyramidal, SOM and PV cells, in addition to the VIPs). Thats exactly what Dylan and team did!

First, we established that attention leads to strong modulation of stimulus response selectivity, and separately, VIP activation leads to strong modulation of pyramidal cell activity. But when we put the two together, there was no interaction. ie, attention and VIP led to independent modulations.

This result was confirmed by silencing VIPs, which left the attentional modulation untouched. So attention signals do not pass through VIP cells. In fact, we found that at the population level, the two modulations were orthogonal.

And because we had measured the activity of VIP, SOM, PV and pyramidals simultaneously, we could show that the changes in different cell classes and their interactions was distinct between attention and VIP modulation.

But how is it possible to have a strong modulation by attention AND by VIP on the SAME cells, but the two not interact? With our amazing collaborators Katharina Wilmes and Claudia Clopath, we found a very specific circuit architecture that can support this property.

Please read and share the preprint, which has much more in there. This was a great team effort led by Dylan Myers-Joseph, and with tremendous contributions from @k47h4, Marian Fernandez-Otero and Claudia Clopath

Show thread

Attention can enhance responses in sensory areas, and this is a pretty fundamental cognitive process, but how does this happen? Check out the latest preprint from our lab which tackles this question, led by the amazing Dylan Myers-Joseph!

biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/20

Please read and share, and see below for summary

Adil Khan boosted

RT @BritishNeuro@twitter.com

The deadline to become a BNA Scholar is TOMORROW!
Apply now to join our third cohort of Scholars
Our programme supports Scholars & helps to improve equity, diversity and inclusion (EDI) in #neuroscience
Info & apply here: ow.ly/xksO50L0xGQ

πŸ¦πŸ”—: twitter.com/BritishNeuro/statu

(11 of 11) This work was a great team effort, driven by Nick Cole
with tremendous contributions from Matt Harvey, Dylan Myers-Joseph and modelling of the behaviour by Aditya Gilra. Feedback welcome! Please RT

Show thread

(10 of 11) If you've made it this far, check out the paper which has some other cool findings, particularly context-switching RL models. Also, I have found that its easy to misunderstand this task, so please reach out if any doubts.

Show thread

(9 of 11) VIP activation almost abolished the mismatch signal! (Since the manipulation was only in the imaging site, not bilateral, this didn't affect behaviour). So VIP cells are key to generating the prediction error signal. This effect (riding on disinhibition by VIPs) constrains circuit models of mismatch
Image

Show thread

(8 of 11) Anyone who's trained a mouse will know, if they don't get it right a few times they will do exploratory licks. Mice with ACC silenced ignore tens of trials of visual stimuli, rewarded if they would lick, but they are stuck in the odour block rule: ignore visual, wait for odour...

And once they do eventually switch, they do near-perfect performance the rest of the block, with ACC still silenced! We also did a fun single-trial Un-silencing experiment to nail this point. So ACC is truly a gate, allowing transitions between cognitive states (and PL is not)

Finally, what is the circuit for comparing prediction and reality? Amazing prior work (Keller lab) shows VIP & SOM interneurons are key. So we activated VIPs while measuring mismatch signals. All-optical method so we are sure the manipulation caused disinhibition as expected

Show thread

(7 of 11) Coming back to the ACC, are these mismatch signals actually needed for switching behaviour? We silenced ACC: the mice showed a major deficit in switching, but perfect accuracy once switched, each time! Neighboring prelimbic didn't have any such effect. This is something to behold ...

Show thread

(6 of 11) If the mismatch signal is so important to behaviour, is it broadcast across most of cortex? we used widefield calcium imaging to map it across the dorsal cortex, and found it to be beautifully restricted to frontal areas (above ACC/PFC). Similar to primates :)

Show thread

(5 of 11) Crucially, the amplitude of this signal is larger when the NEXT trial is going to be a correct one-shot transition (compared to transitions where it makes 2 or 3 errors before switching). The mismatch amplitude matters to upcoming behaviour. An RL model showed cool similarities

Show thread

(4 of 11) ~10% of all ACC neurons show this, a big number considering PFC responses. And we didn't see this signal by chance/movement artefacts, eg.identical recordings from V1 didn't show it. Also this signal is different based on where cells project to: mismatch signals are excluded from ACC neurons projecting to the striatum

Show thread

(3 of 11) This is not just a surprise, like oddball tasks. This is a prediction mismatch which has demonstrable behavioural consequences - the mice switch behaviour using it. A complete switch in mental rules happens at a well defined moment - the mouse proves this to us by its behaviour

We recorded from the ACC, and aligned activity to the moment the odour was expected to arrive but didn't, and found neurons with a clear response - to the NON occurrence of an expected odour. Remember nothing happened at this moment - only the expectation violation.

Show thread

(2 of 11) When they expected an odour (rule2) which didn't arrive, they switched rules, and started responding based on rule 1, the very next trial, and stayed very accurate for the rest of the block, and did this over and over allowing us to zoom in on the rare moment of prediction mismatch!

Show thread

(1 of 11) How does the brain change its mind? Please check out the first preprint from the Khan lab! The PhD work of the incredible Nick Cole. We find that mice can switch 'one-shot' between distinct cognitive rules, driven by prediction-error signals in the ACC.
biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/20

Tootprint:
Animals need to hold abstract rules or plans in mind and switch between them when appropriate. Imagine you walk up to your supermarket but the automatic doors don't open. This brief moment is often enough to make you mentally switch to an alternative shopping plan, and walk away to the next shop.

What happened at the moment of the door not opening? You switched (very fast) between two abstracts plans. This switching is a key cognitive ability. What signals in the brain trigger this transition? Lots of amazing prior work shows that the Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) is key

One suggestion is that the ACC compares a prediction (door should open) to observation (it didn't) and the mismatch makes you switch plans. This is challenging to test! An animal must demonstrably hold a rule in mind, and when the rule prediction is violated, it must switch the plan held in mind, but...

If an animal takes multiple trials to switch (usually the case) and shows intermediate behaviour for a few trials, its difficult to assign activity in the brain precisely to the cognitive rule switch. And subsequently its difficult to identify circuit mechanisms behind the mismatch computation.

Nick got mice to do something impressive: Blocks of highly accurate visual discrimination (Rule1) followed by highly accurate odour discrimination while ignoring the same visual stimuli (Rule2), and the block transition was triggered by just one experience of an expectation violation

I'm Adil Khan, a neuroscientist and group leader at King's College London, studying how the brain generates flexible behaviour. I'm interested in a neural circuit level understanding of cognition. My research involves recording from and manipulating neurons in mice performing cognitive tasks, to address basic questions about the nature of learning, attention and task-switching

Qoto Mastodon

QOTO: Question Others to Teach Ourselves
An inclusive, Academic Freedom, instance
All cultures welcome.
Hate speech and harassment strictly forbidden.